Go to dissertation
process page.
Eric Miller,
Ph.D. candidate, Folklore Program,
University of Pennsylvania (Philadelphia).
Summary of dissertation research project.
“Verbal Play and Language Learning”
2003-4
At this moment in history, many of the world’s traditional
verbal arts
are in danger of becoming extinct. Even among people living in
and
around forest areas, many of the traditional songs are nearly
forgotten.
The old people today are the last generation that has been raised
without
the presence of electricity and mass media such as television.
Just
as it has been shown that bio-diversity in the environment is
beneficial
for human beings, the same is true for cultural diversity.
Therefore,
it is crucial to seize the moment and to collect the traditional verbal
arts before they are lost. Once the traditional verbal arts are
collected,
people in future generations – both inside and outside the group – will
be able to study, enjoy, and learn from them.
Verbal arts are especially alive when they are being used to
help educate
the young. Thus, I am hoping to collect stories, proverbs, and
lullabies
that adults sing and tell to children. I would also collect
songs,
verbal games, riddles, puns, and tongue-twisters that children share
amongst
themselves.
A primary research question of this dissertation is, "How
might verbal
play assist in the child's language learning process?" It is my
hypothesis
that verbal play indeed does help with language learning, and I will be
searching for scientific proof to support this claim. I will pay
special attention to the occurrences of repetition with variation
(including substitution, modification, and addition
of verbal elements from one phrase or sentence to the next) in
children’s
exploring and practicing of spoken language. I will also observe
the uses of melody, rhythm, and gesture, and how these features may
improve
verbal memory (Jakobson 1960; Kirshenblatt-Gimblett 1976).
Throughout,
I will consider issues relating to verbal play and phonetics
(sounds),
semantics
(words), and syntax (sentence structure) in the experience of
the
growing child.
A central problem to be investigated will be, “What are
successful methods
of teaching and learning spoken language and verbal arts?” The
fieldwork
will help me to develop a general method, utilizing verbal play and
games,
for teaching spoken language (Maley and Duff 1982). This method
would
function for teaching adults as well as children.
The language learning process is often considered to involve
three successive
stages: the afore-mentioned phonetics, semantics, and syntax
(Kirshenblatt-Gimblett
and Sherzer). Related to syntax is accidence, a
language’s
rules for the forms words take when they are combined to make phrases
and
sentences. Together, phonetics, syntax, and accidence constitute
a language’s grammar (Oxford English Dictionary). It
should
be kept in mind that “while language acquisition studies generally
indicate
that the child is syntactically competent by about the age of four
years,
phonological dominance is maintained long after the child is
theoretically
competent semantically and syntactically” (Sanches and
Kirshenblatt-Gimblett
105). In other words, children are often most sensitive to the
sounds
of speech, rather than to the abstract meanings created by words and
sentences.
The following schedule varies greatly from child to child
(Garvey; Farb):
Infants may begin to coo and babble shortly after
birth.
At six months, reduplicative babbling is common. At eight
months, "children use intonation patterns similar to those changes in
pitch
heard in adult exclamations and questions" (Farb 10). At 12
months,
single-syllable words are sometimes uttered. At 18 months, a
child's
vocabulary is often between three and 50 words, and the child may be
able
to utter simple stock phrases, such as to request something. It
is
approximately at this time that the "naming explosion" or "word spurt"
may begin. The naming explosion tends to occur at the same time
as
the onset of productive syntax.
At 24 months, a child may be able to name most of the
physical objects
he comes into contact with on a daily basis, and two- and three-word
utterances
are common. For many children, 36 months marks the end of the
naming
explosion period: now the child's vocabulary may be 1,000 words.
At 48 months, typically the child has mastered most of the syntactical
structures of the language. By this time, the development of the
child's cognitive architecture -- involving such distinctions as those,
for example, between the name of a dog, the abstract category, dog, and
the abstract category, animal -- is also well underway. At this
time,
the child can also engage in many interactional routines,
especially
those involving questions-and-answers.
Numerous theories have been created to account for how
children learn
language. Among these theories are B. F. Skinner's theory of Behaviorism,
and Noam Chomsky's theory of a Universal Grammar. To
briefly
summarize these theories:
Skinner's theory of Behaviorism posits that all learning
results from
environmental conditioning, from positive and negative feedback
(rewards
and punishments). These reinforcements inculcate habits in the
learner.
Behaviorism states that all learning results from memorization of what
has been presented from without.
Chomsky's theory of a Universal Grammar refutes Skinner's
claim, instead
positing that humans are born with "innate behavior patterns, and
tendencies
to learn in specific ways" (Chomsky 57). People are born with a
"grammatical
sense" (Chomsky 56), a "built-in structure of an information-processing
(hypothesis-forming) system [which] enables them to arrive at the
grammar
of a language from the available data at the time" (Chomsky 58).
According to this theory, there are fundamental internal processes at
work
in each human, quite independent of feedback from the
environment.
"The fact that all normal children acquire essentially comparable
grammars
of great complexity with remarkable rapidity suggests that human beings
are somehow specially designed to do this, with data-handling ability
of
an unknown character and complexity" (Chomsky 57).
Another theory of language that should be mentioned in this
context
is Edward Sapir's and Benjamin Whorf's Sapir-Whorf theory of
language.
According to this theory, "Language is not merely a reproducing
instrument
for voicing ideas, but rather is itself the shaper of ideas. We
dissect
nature along the lines laid down by our native language" (as cited in
Farb
180). That is, a language guides its users into perceiving,
thinking
about, and expressing reality in well-worn channels. Languages
create
these constraints simply through the limited options they offer, in
terms
of such elements as words (or lack of them), gender and verb tense
forms,
and the ways that words combine and interact.
India has been chosen for this research project because
India is very
rich in traditional verbal arts. Tamil Nadu has been chosen
because
of the beauty and ancient history of the Tamil language and
culture.
Finally, the southernmost Western Ghats has been chosen as the specific
fieldwork location because traditional verbal arts are best studied
where
they continue to function as primary sources of entertainment, far from
the centers of mass media production and consumption.
Having chosen the fieldwork location, I discovered that the
Kanikaran
people live there (Stephen 1997; Tharmaraj 1994). Two special
features
of Kanikaran language are:
Firstly: A people’s language is influenced by the
environment.
The Kanikaran people mostly live in forest areas. Thus, their
language
is likely to be affected in many ways by the natural
elements.
My preliminary impression is that sounds and movements of animals are
often
imitated in Kanikaran children's songs and games; that animals,
vegetation,
weather, and geographical landmarks play important roles in Kanikaran
folksongs
and folktales; and that everyday Kanikaran language contains many
expressions
about the natural elements.
Secondly: Most of the Kanikaran people speak Tamil and
live in
Tamil Nadu, and it is here that I hope to do the great majority of my
fieldwork.
(I have studied Tamil in university classrooms in the USA and India for
a number of years, and I am working hard to become fluent in the spoken
language.) However, some Kanikaran people also live in
neighboring
areas of Kerala and speak Malayalam. Thus, the group’s language
features
a mixture of Tamil and Malayalam languages, at times forming a hybrid,
combination language. There is even a slang hybrid mode of
language,
known as “Kani pasi.” Although some literary purists may look
down
on such hybrid oral languages, many scholars find them very interesting
(Schieffelin and Ochs 1986).
The research project will aspire to work within the
tradition of the
Ethnography
of Speaking. In 1962, Dell Hymes founded this
interdisciplinary
field, calling upon scholars to survey the full range of speech
activities
in the communities they would study (Hymes 1962). Extended to
communication
in general, the field has also come to be known as the Ethnography
of
Communication (Gumperz and Hymes 1972).
Another field of study that grew out of Hymes’ work, and
which pays
close attention to the process of communication, is known as the Performance-centered
Approach to Folklore (Paredes and Bauman 1972). The cohort of
folklorists that developed this approach, beginning in the late 60’s,
was
primarily concerned with the styles of performance; the tones and
rhythms
of voice; body language; and teller-listener interactions. They
paid
close attention to the social situations in which the verbal arts were
performed.
Two methodologies that will be used in the course of the
fieldwork project
are that I will visit schools (regarding older children) and families
(regarding
younger children). The data to be collected will be that which
pertains
to the teaching and performing of oral language and verbal arts.
When possible, I will interview both teachers and learners of language
and verbal arts, regarding techniques of teaching and learning.
The language teaching and storytelling by grandmothers will
come under
special attention, as elderly women in rural India tend to be
especially
knowledgeable about various verbal arts, including the chanting and
singing
of lullabies and laments (Venugopal 1996).
Throughout the fieldwork project, the following four-step
transcription-translation
process will be utilized:
1) Tamil.
2) English transliteration, that is, sounds
(this is the phonetic level).
3) English words (this is the semantic level).
4) English sentences (this is the syntax level).
This four-step system will enable the English-literate reader to see,
in
a single glance, what English sounds and words the Tamil script refers
to, syllable for syllable. Following in the tradition of ethnopoetics,
scoring techniques will be used to indicate voice tone, melody, and
rhythm
(Tedlock 1992). Transcriptions will also include notation of
gesture
(Birdwhistell 1970).
In summary: The above-described research project
represents a
golden opportunity to rescue from possible oblivion many of the
Kanikaran
people’s traditional verbal arts. It would also make possible,
for
the present researcher and for future researchers, the study of the
Kanikaran
people’s methods of teaching those verbal arts to the young, and of
teaching
the language itself. The successful completion of the project
would
most certainly contribute to the word’s storehouse of linguistic
treasures.
Bibliography
Bavin, Edith. 1995. “Language Acquisition in
Crosslinguistic
Perspective.” Annual Review of Anthropology 24: 373-96.
Bloom, Paul, ed. 1994. Language Acquisition:
Core Readings.
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Birdwhistell, Ray. 1970. Kinesics and Context:
Essays on
Body Motion Communication. Philadelphia: U. of Pennsylvania
Press.
Chomsky, Noam. 1959. “Verbal Behavior, by B. F.
Skinner”
(Review). Language 35 (1): 26-58.
Ervin-Trip, Susan, and Claudia Mitchell-Kernan.
1977. “Introduction.”
In: Child Discourse, edited by the authors, NY: Academic Press, pp.
1-23.
Farb, Peter. 1973. Word Play: What Happens When
People Talk.
NY: Knopf.
Fernald, Anne. 1994. “Human Maternal
Vocalizations to Infants
as Biologically Relevant Signals: An Evolutionary Perspective.”
In:
Language Acquisition: Core Readings, P. Bloom, ed., Cambridge, Mass.:
MIT
Press, pp. 51-94.
Garvey, Catherine. 1977. “Playing with Language
and Speech.”
In: Child Discourse, S. Ervin-Trip and C. Mitchell-Kernan, eds., NY:
Academic
Press, pp. 27-47.
Gleason, Jean Berko, and Sandra Weintraub. 1976.
“The Acquisition
of Routines in Child Language." Language In Society 5:
129-36.
Gumperz, John, and Dell Hymes, eds. 1972.
Directions in
Sociolinguistics: The Ethnography of Communication. NY: Holt,
Rinehart,
and Winston.
Iwamura, Susan Grohs. 1980. The Verbal Games of
Pre-school
Children. NY: St. Martin's Press.
Huizinga, Johan. 1949. Homo Ludens: a Study of
the Play-element
in Culture. Trans. by R. F. C. Hull. London: Routledge
&
K. Paul.
Hymes, Dell. 1962. “The Ethnography of
Speaking.”
In: Anthropology and Human Behavior, T. Gladwin and W. Sturtevant,
eds.,
Washington: Anthropological Society
of Washington, pp. 15-53.
Hymes, Dell. 1973. “The Scope of
Sociolinguistics.” In:
Sociolinguistics: State and Prospect, R. W. Shuy, ed., Washington,
D.C.:
Georgetown U. Press, pp. 313-33.
Jakobson, Roman. 1960. “Linguistics and
Poetics.”
In: Style in Language, T. Sebeok, ed., Cambridge, MA: Massachusetts
Institute
of Technology, pp. 350-377.
Jones, Bessie, and Bess Lomax Hawes. 1987
(1972). Step It
Down: Games, Plays, Songs, and Stories from the Afro-American
Heritage.
Athens: U. of Georgia Press.
Keenan, Elinor Ochs. 1977. “Making It Last:
Repetition in
Children's Discourse.” In: Child Discourse, S. Ervin-Trip and C.
Mitchell-Kernan, eds., NY: Academic Press, pp. 125-38.
Kirshenblatt-Gimblett, Barbara, and Joel Sherzer.
1976.
“Introduction.” In: Speech Play: Research and Resources for
Studying
Linguistic Creativity, B. Kirshenblatt-Gimblett, ed.,
Philadelphia
: U. of Pennsylvania Press, pp. 1-16.
Kirshenblatt-Gimblett, Barbara, ed. 1976. Speech
Play: Research
and Resources for Studying Linguistic Creativity. Philadelphia:
U.
of Pennsylvania Press.
Larsen-Freeman, Diane. 2000. Techniques and
Principles in
Language Teaching (2nd Ed.). Oxford: Oxford U. Press.
Lightbown, Patsy, and Nina Spada. 1993. How
Languages are
Learned. Oxford: Oxford U. Press.
Maley, Alan, and Alan Duff. 1982. Drama
Techniques in Language
Learning. (Series: Cambridge Handbooks for Language
Teachers.)
Cambridge: Cambridge U. Press.
Ochs, Elinor. 1998. Culture and Language
Development: Language
Acquisition and Language Socialization in a Samoan Village.
Cambridge:
Cambridge U. Press.
Opie, Iona, and Peter Opie. 1985. The Singing
Game.
Oxford: Oxford U. Press.
Oxford English Dictionary Online. 2003.
Paredes, Américo, and Richard Bauman, eds.
1972. Towards
New Perspectives in Folklore. Austin: U. of Texas Press.
Pinker, Steven. 2000. The Language Instinct: How
the Mind
Creates Language. NY: Perennial Classics.
Sanches, Mary, and Barbara Kirshenblatt-Gimblett.
1976.
“Children's Traditional Speech Play and Child Language.” In:
Speech
Play: Research and Resources for Studying Linguistic Creativity, B.
Kirshenblatt-Gimblett,
ed., Philadelphia: U. of Pennsylvania Press, pp. 65-110.
Schieffelin, Bambi, and Elinor Ochs, eds. 1986.
Language
Socialization Across Cultures. Cambridge: Cambridge U.
Press.
Schwartzman, Helen. 1978. Transformations: The
Anthropology
of Children’s Play. NY: Plenum Press.
Stephen, G. 1997. Kokkarai: Life and Culture of
the Kanikarar.
Dept. of Tamil Studies, Manomaniyam Sundaranar University,
Sutton-Smith, Brian, ed. 1979. Play and
Learning.
NY: Gardner Press.
Sutton-Smith, Brian. 1997. The Ambiguity of
Play.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard U. Press.
Tedlock, Dennis. 1983. The Spoken Word and the
Work of Interpretation.
Philadelphia: U. of Pennsylvania Press.
Tharmaraj, Y. 1994. Folklore of the
Kanikarans.
Dissertation, Dept. of Tamil, University of Kerala, Trivandrum.
Venugopal, Saraswathi. 1996. Folkloristic
Refractions in
the Tamil World. Madurai: Tamarai Press.
Widdowson, J. D. A. 2001. “Rhythm, Repetition,
and Rhetoric:
Learning Language in the School Playground.” In: Play Today in
the
Primary School Playground, J. Bishop and M. Curtis, eds., Philadelphia:
Open U. Press, pp. 135-51.
Williamson, Susan. 1979. Tamil Baby Talk: A
Cross-Cultural
Study. Dissertation, South Asia Regional Studies, U. of
Pennsylvania.